ABSTRACT
Learning to speak involves both mastering the requisite articulatory gestures of one’s native language and learning to coordinate those gestures according to the rules of the language. Voice onset time (VOT) acquisition illustrates this point: The child must learn to produce the necessary upper vocal tract and laryngeal gestures and to coordinate them with very precise timing. This longitudinal study examined the acquisition of English VOT by audiotaping seven children at intervals from first words (around ) to the appearance of three-word sentences (around ) in spontaneous speech. Words with initial stops were excerpted, and (1) the numbers of words produced with intended voiced and voiceless initial stops were counted; (2) VOT was measured; and (3) within-child standard deviations of VOT were measured. Results showed that children (1) initially avoided saying words with voiceless initial stops, (2) initially did not delay the onset of the laryngeal adduction relative to the release of closure as long as adults do for voiceless stops, and (3) were more variable in VOT for voiceless than for voiced stops. Overall these results support a model of acquisition that focuses on the mastery of gestural coordination as opposed to the acquisition of segmental contrasts.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This work was supported by research Grant No. R01 DC00633 from the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, the National Institutes of Health, to S.N. We thank Carol Manning and Gina Meyer for their assistance in data collection and digitizing. Portions of this work were presented at the 147th Meeting of the Acoustical Society of America, New York, May 2004.
- 1. Allen, G. D. (1985). “How the young French child avoids the pre-voicing problem for word-initial voiced stops,” J. Child Lang 12, 37–46. Google ScholarCrossref
- 2. Barton, D., and Macken, M. A. (1980). “An instrumental analysis of the voicing contrast in word-initial stops in the speech of four-year-old English-speaking children,” Lang Speech 23, 159–169. Google ScholarCrossref
- 3. Bean, C. H. (1932). “An unusual opportunity to investigate the psychology of language,” J. Genet. Psychol. 40, 181–202. Google Scholar
- 4. BMDP Statistical Software (1990). BMDP Statistical Software, Inc., Los Angeles. Google Scholar
- 5. Browman, C. P., and Goldstein, L. (1989). “Articulatory gestures as phonological units,” Phonology 6, 201–251. Google ScholarCrossref
- 6. Bruner, J. S. (1973). “Organization of early skilled action,” Child Dev. 44, 1–11. Google ScholarCrossref
- 7. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed. (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ). Google Scholar
- 8. de Boysson–Bardies, B., Sagart, L., Halle, P., and Durand, C. (1986). “Acoustic investigation of crosslinguistic variability in babbling,” in Precursors of Early Speech, edited by B. Lindblom and R. Zetterström (Stockton, New York), pp. 113–126. Google ScholarCrossref
- 9. Dobrich, W., and Scarborough, H. S. (1992). “Phonological characteristics of words young children try to say,” J. Child Lang 19, 597–616. Google ScholarCrossref
- 10. Eilers, R., Oller, D. K., and Benito-Garcia, C. R. (1984). “The acquisition of voicing contrasts in Spanish and English learning infants and children: A longitudinal study,” J. Child Lang 11, 313–336. Google ScholarCrossref
- 11. Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P. W., and Vigorito, J. (1971). “Speech perception in infants,” Science 171, 303–306. Google ScholarCrossref
- 12. Ferguson, C. A., and Farwell, C. B. (1975). “Words and sounds in early language acquisition,” Language 51, 419–439. Google ScholarCrossref
- 13. Gilbert, J. H. (1977). “A voice onset time analysis of apical stop production in -olds,” J. Child Lang 4, 103–110. Google ScholarCrossref
- 14. Goffman, L., and Smith, A. (1999). “Development and phonetic differentiation of speech movement patterns,” J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 25, 649–660. Google ScholarCrossref
- 15. Goodell, E. W., and Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1991). “Articulatory organization of early words: From syllable to phoneme,” in Proceedings of the XIIth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (Aix-en-Provence), pp. 166–169. Google Scholar
- 16. Goodell, E. W., and Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1993). “Acoustic evidence for the development of gestural coordination in the speech of -olds: A longitudinal study,” J. Speech Hear. Res. 36, 707–727. Google ScholarCrossref
- 17. Green, J. R., Moore, C. A., Higashikawa, M., and Steeve, R. W. (2000). “The physiologic development of speech motor coordination: Lip and jaw coordination,” J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 43, 239–255. Google ScholarCrossref
- 18. Hodge, M. M. (1990). “Measuring coarticulation in children’s speech: Quirks and questions,” Paper presented at the Child Phonology Conference, Madison, WI, May. Google Scholar
- 19. Jakobson, R. (1941). Kindersprache, Aphasie und Allgemeine Lautgesetze. Reprinted as Child Language, Aphasia, and Phonological Universals, 1968. Mouton, The Hague. Google Scholar
- 20. Kehoe, M. M., Lleo, C., and Rakow, M. (2004). “Voice onset time in bilingual German-Spanish children,” Bilingualism: Lang. Cognit. 7, 71–88. Google ScholarCrossref
- 21. Kelso, J. A., Tuller, B., Vatikiotis-Bateson, E., and Fowler, C. A. (1984). “Functionally specific articulatory cooperation following jaw perturbations during speech: Evidence for coordinative structures,” J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.10.6.812 10, 812–832. Google ScholarCrossref
- 22. Kent, R. D. (1976). “Tutorial: Anatomical and neuromuscular maturation of the speech mechanism: Evidence from acoustic studies,” J. Speech Hear. Res. 19, 421–445. Google ScholarCrossref
- 23. Kent, R. D. (1983). “The segmental organization of speech,” in The Production of Speech, edited by P. F. MacNeilage (Springer, New York), pp. 57–89. Google ScholarCrossref
- 24. Kewley-Port, D., and Preston, M. S. (1974). “Early apical stop production: A voice onset time analysis,” J. Phonetics 2, 195–210. Google Scholar
- 25. Koenig, L. L. (2000). “Laryngeal factors in voiceless consonant production in men, women, and -olds,” J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 43, 1211–1228. Google ScholarCrossref
- 26. Lindblom, B. (1990). “Explaining phonetic variation: A sketch of the H&H theory,” in Speech Production and Speech Modeling, edited by J. W. Hardcastle and A. Marchal (Kluwer Academic, The Netherlands), pp. 403–439. Google ScholarCrossref
- 27. Lisker, L., and Abramson, A. S. (1964). “A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements,” Word 20, 384–422. Google ScholarCrossref
- 28. Locke, J. L. (1980). “Mechanisms of phonological development in children: Maintenance, learning, and loss,” in Papers from the Sixteenth Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society (Chicago Linguistic Society, Chicago), pp. 220–238. Google Scholar
- 29. Locke, J. L. (1983). Phonological Acquisition and Change (Academic, New York). Google Scholar
- 30. Löfqvist, A., McGarr, N. S., and Honda, K. (1984). “Laryngeal muscles and articulatory control,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.391278 76, 951–954. Google ScholarScitation
- 31. Löfqvist, A., and Yoshioka, H. (1980). “Laryngeal activity in Swedish obstruent clusters,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.384774 68, 792–801. Google ScholarScitation
- 32. Macken, M. A., and Barton, D. (1980a). “The acquisition of the voicing contrast in English: A study of voice onset time in word-initial stop consonants,” J. Child Lang 7, 41–74. Google ScholarCrossref
- 33. Macken, M. A., and Barton, D. (1980b). “The acquisition of the voicing contrast in Spanish: a phonetic and phonological study of word-initial stop consonants,” J. Child Lang 7, 433–478. Google ScholarCrossref
- 34. MacNeilage, P. F., and Davis, B. (1991). “Acquisition of speech production: Frames, then content,” in Attention and Performance XIII, edited by M. Jeannerod (Erlbaum, New York), pp. 453–476. Google Scholar
- 35. MacNeilage, P. F., and Davis, B. L. (1993). “Motor explanations of babbling and early speech patterns,” in Developmental Neurocognition: Speech and Face Processing in the First Year of Life, edited by B. de Boysson-Bardies, S. Schonen, P. Jusczyk, P. MacNeilage, and J. Morton (Kluwer Academic, Dordrecht), pp. 341–352. Google ScholarCrossref
- 36. McGowan, R. S., Nittrouer, S., and Manning, C. J. (2004). “Development of [ɹ] in young, midwestern, American children,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1642624 115, 871–884. Google ScholarScitation, ISI
- 37. Menn, L. (1978). “Phonological units in beginning speech,” in Syllables and Segments, edited by A. Bell and J. B. Hooper (North-Holland, Amsterdam), pp. 157–172. Google Scholar
- 38. Menn, L. (1983). “Development of articulatory, phonetic and phonological capabilities,” in Language Production: Development, Writing and Other Language Processes, edited by B. Butterworth (Academic, New York), pp. 3–50. Google Scholar
- 39. Menyuk, P., and Klatt, M. (1975). “Voice onset time in consonant cluster production by children and adults,” J. Child Lang 2, 223–231. Google ScholarCrossref
- 40. Munhall, K. G. (1985). “An examination of intra-articulator relative timing,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.392790 78, 1548–1553. Google ScholarScitation
- 41. Nittrouer, S. (1991). “Phase relations of jaw and tongue tip movements in the production of VCV utterances,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.401661 90, 1806–1815. Google ScholarScitation, ISI
- 42. Nittrouer, S. (1993). “The emergence of mature gestural patterns is not uniform: Evidence from an acoustic study,” J. Speech Hear. Res. 36, 959–972. Google ScholarCrossref
- 43. Nittrouer, S., Estee, S., Lowenstein, J. H., and Smith, J. (2005). “The emergence of mature gestural patterns in the production of voiceless and voiced word-final stops,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1828474 117, 351–364. Google ScholarScitation, ISI
- 44. Nittrouer, S., Munhall, K., Kelso, J. A., Tuller, B., and Harris, K. S. (1988). “Patterns of interarticulator phasing and their relation to linguistic structure,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.397180 84, 1653–1661. Google ScholarScitation
- 45. Nittrouer, S., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and McGowan, R. S. (1989). “The emergence of phonetic segments: Evidence from the spectral structure of fricative-vowel syllables spoken by children and adults,” J. Speech Hear. Res. 32, 120–132. Google ScholarCrossref
- 46. Nittrouer, S., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Neely, S. T. (1996). “How children learn to organize their speech gestures: Further evidence from fricative-vowel syllables,” J. Speech Hear. Res. 39, 379–389. Google ScholarCrossref
- 47. Oller, D. K., and Eilers, R. E. (1982). “Similarity of babbling in Spanish- and English-learning babies,” J. Child Lang 9, 565–577. Google ScholarCrossref
- 48. Osgood, C. E. (1953). Method and Theory in Experimental Psychology (Oxford University Press, New York). Google Scholar
- 49. Prather, E. M., Hedrick, D. L., and Kern, C. A. (1975). “Articulation development in children aged two to four years,” J. Speech Hear Disord. 40, 179–191. Google ScholarCrossref
- 50. Schwartz, R. G., and Leonard, L. B. (1982). “Do children pick and choose: An examination of phonological selection and avoidance in early lexical acquisition,” J. Child Lang 9, 319–336. Google ScholarCrossref
- 51. Schwartz, R. G., Leonard, L. B., Loeb, D. M., and Swanson, L. A. (1987). “Attempted sounds are sometimes not: An expanded view of phonological selection and avoidance.” J. Child Lang 14, 411–418. Google ScholarCrossref
- 52. Scobbie, J. M., Gibbon, F., Hardcastle, W. J., and Fletcher, P. (2000). “Covert contrast as a stage in the acquisition of phonetics and phonology,” in Papers in Laboratory Phonology V: Language Acquisition and the Lexicon, edited by Michael Broe and Janet Pierrehumbert (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge), pp. 194–207. Google Scholar
- 53. Sjölander, K., and Beskow, J. (2000). “Wavesurfer: An open source speech tool,” Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Spoken Language Processing, Vol. 4, 464–467. Google Scholar
- 54. Smith, A., and Goffman, L. (1998). “Stability and patterning of speech movement sequences in children and adults,” J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 41, 18–30. Google ScholarCrossref
- 55. Smith, B. L., and McLean-Muse, A. (1986). “Articulatory movement characteristics of labial consonant productions by children and adults,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.394383 80, 1321–1328. Google ScholarScitation
- 56. Smith, B. L., Sugarman, M. D., and Long, S. H. (1983). “Experimental manipulation of speaking rate for studying temporal variability in children’s speech,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.389860 74, 744–749. Google ScholarScitation
- 57. Snow, D. (1997). “Children’s acquisition of speech timing in English: A comparative study of voice onset time and final syllable vowel lengthening,” J. Child Lang 24, 35–56. Google ScholarCrossref
- 58. Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1987). “The phoneme as a perceptuomotor structure,” in Language Perception and Production: Relationships Between Listening, Speaking, Reading, and Writing, edited by A. Allport, D. G. MacKay, W. Prinz, and E. Scheerer (Academic, Orlando), pp. 67–84. Google Scholar
- 59. Tyler, A. A., and Saxman, J. H. (1991). “Initial voicing contrast acquisition in normal and phonologically disordered children,” Appl. Psycholinguist. 12, 453–479. Google ScholarCrossref
- 60. Velten, H. V. (1943). “The growth of phonemic and lexical patterns in infant language,” Language 19, 281–292. Google ScholarCrossref
- 61. Vihman, M. M., and Velleman, S. L. (1989). “Phonological reorganization: A case study,” Lang Speech 32, 149–170. Google ScholarCrossref
- 62. Vihman, M. M., and Velleman, S. L. (2000). “The construction of a first phonology,” Phonetica 57, 255–266. Google ScholarCrossref
- 63. Werker, J. F., and Tees, R. C. (1999). “Influences on infant speech processing: Toward a new synthesis,” Annu. Rev. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.509 50, 509–535. Google ScholarCrossref
- 64. Whalen, D. H., Levitt, A. G., and Goldstein, L. M. (2007). “VOT in the babbling of French- and English-learning infants,” J. Phonetics 35, 341–352. Google ScholarCrossref
- 65. Zlatin, M. A., and Koenigsknecht, R. A. (1976). “Development of the voicing contrast: A comparison of voice onset time in stop perception and production,” J. Speech Hear. Res. 19, 93–111. Google ScholarCrossref
Please Note: The number of views represents the full text views from December 2016 to date. Article views prior to December 2016 are not included.