No Access Submitted: 23 May 2008 Accepted: 16 November 2008 Published Online: 04 February 2009
The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 125, 1082 (2009); https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3050271
more...View Affiliations
View Contributors
  • Laurent Demany
  • Robert P. Carlyon
  • Catherine Semal
Sek and Moore [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 106, 351–359 (1999)] and Lyzenga et al. [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 116, 491–501 (2004)] found that the just-noticeable frequency difference between two pure tones relatively close in time is smaller when these tones are smoothly connected by a frequency glide than when they are separated by a silent interval. This “glide effect” was interpreted as evidence that frequency glides can be detected by a specific auditory mechanism, not involved in the detection of discrete, time-delayed frequency changes. Lyzenga et al. argued in addition that the glide-detection mechanism provides little information on the direction of frequency changes near their detection threshold. The first experiment reported here confirms the existence of the glide effect, but also shows that it disappears when the glide is not connected smoothly to the neighboring steady tones. A second experiment demonstrates that the direction of a 750ms frequency glide can be perceptually identified as soon as the glide is detectable. These results, and some other observations, lead to a new interpretation of the glide effect, and to the conclusion that continuous frequency changes may be detected in the same manner as discrete frequency changes.
The authors are grateful to Marie Dejos and Maialen Erviti for their precious collaboration. They also thank Johannes Lyzenga and Brian Moore for beneficial discussions.
  1. 1. Carlyon, R. P., Micheyl, C., Deeks, J. M., and Moore, B. C. J. (2004). “Auditory processing of real and illusory changes in FM phase,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1811474 116, 3629–3639. Google ScholarScitation
  2. 2. Cusack, R., and Carlyon, R. P. (2003). “Perceptual asymmetries in audition,” J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.29.3.713 26, 713–725. Google ScholarCrossref
  3. 3. Demany, L., Pressnitzer, D., and Semal, C. (2008). “On the binding of successive tones: Implicit versus explicit pitch comparisons,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 123, 3049. Google ScholarScitation
  4. 4. Demany, L., and Ramos, C. (2005). “On the binding of successive sounds: Perceiving shifts in nonperceived pitches,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1850209 117, 833–841. Google ScholarScitation, ISI
  5. 5. Demany, L., and Ramos, C. (2007). “A paradoxical aspect of auditory change detection,” in Hearing—From Sensory Processing to Perception, edited by B. Kollmeier, G. Klump, V. Hohmann, U. Langemann, M. Mauermann, S. Uppenkamp, and J. Verhey (Springer, Heidelberg), pp. 313–321. Google ScholarCrossref
  6. 6. Demany, L., and Semal, C. (1989). “Detection thresholds for sinusoidal frequency modulation,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.397460 85, 1295–1301. Google ScholarScitation, ISI
  7. 7. Dooley, G. J., and Moore, B. C. J. (1988). “Detection of linear frequency glides as a function of frequency and duration,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.397048 84, 2045–2057. Google ScholarScitation
  8. 8. Green, D. M., and Swets, J. A. (1974). Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics (Krieger, New York). Google Scholar
  9. 9. Hartmann, W. M. (1997). Signals, Sound, and Sensation (AIP, Woodbury, New York). Google Scholar
  10. 10. Hartmann, W. M., and Klein, M. A. (1980). “Theory of frequency modulation detection for low modulation frequencies,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.383972 67, 935–946. Google ScholarScitation, ISI
  11. 11. Kaernbach, C. (1991). “Simple adaptive testing with the weighted up-down method,” Percept. Psychophys. 49, 227–229. Google ScholarCrossref
  12. 12. Kay, R. H. (1982). “Hearing of modulation in sounds,” Physiol. Rev. 62, 894–975. Google ScholarCrossref, ISI
  13. 13. Lyzenga, J., Carlyon, R. P., and Moore, B. C. J. (2004). “The effects of real and illusory glides on pure-tone frequency discrimination,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1756616 116, 491–501. Google ScholarScitation
  14. 14. Madden, J. P., and Fire, K. M. (1997). “Detection and discrimination of frequency glides as a function of direction, duration, frequency span, and center frequency,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.420346 102, 2920–2924. Google ScholarScitation
  15. 15. Micheyl, C., Kaernbach, C., and Demany, L. (2008). “An evaluation of psychophysical models of auditory change perception,” Psychol. Rev., 115, 1069–1083. Google ScholarCrossref
  16. 16. Moody, D. B., Cole, D., Davidson, L. M., and Stebbins, W. C. (1984). “Evidence for a reappraisal of the psychophysical selective adaptation paradigm,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.391399 76, 1076–1079. Google ScholarScitation
  17. 17. Moore, B. C. J. (1973). “Frequency difference limens for short-duration tones,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1913640 54, 610–619. Google ScholarScitation, ISI
  18. 18. Moore, B. C. J. (2004). An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, 5th ed. (Elsevier, Amsterdam). Google Scholar
  19. 19. Nabelek, I. V., and Hirsh, I. J. (1969). “On the discrimination of frequency transitions,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1911631 45, 1510–1519. Google ScholarScitation
  20. 20. Sek, A., and Moore, B. C. J. (1999). “Discrimination of frequency steps linked by glides of various durations,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.427061 106, 351–359. Google ScholarScitation
  21. 21. Semal, C., and Demany, L. (2006). “Individual differences in the sensitivity to pitch direction,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2357708 120, 3907–3915. Google ScholarScitation, ISI
  22. 22. Sergeant, R. L., and Harris, J. D. (1962). “Sensitivity to unidirectional frequency modulation,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1909065 34, 1625–1628. Google ScholarScitation
  23. 23. Wakefield, G. H., and Viemeister, N. F. (1984). “Selective adaptation to linear frequency-modulated sweeps: Evidence for direction-specific FM channels?,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.390808 75, 1588–1592. Google ScholarScitation
  24. 24. Whitfield, I. C., and Evans, E. F. (1965). “Responses of auditory cortical neurons to stimuli of changing frequency,” J. Neurophysiol. 28, 655–672. Google ScholarCrossref
  25. 25. Zhang, L. I., Tan, A. Y. Y., Schreiner, C. E., and Merzenich, M. M. (2003). “Topography and synaptic shaping of direction selectivity in primary auditory cortex,” Nature (London) https://doi.org/10.1038/nature01796 424, 201–205. Google ScholarCrossref
  1. © 2009 Acoustical Society of America..